收藏 分享(赏)

卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf

上传人:文库大宝贝 文档编号:6337515 上传时间:2022-08-03 格式:PDF 页数:9 大小:230.19KB
下载 相关 举报
卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf_第1页
第1页 / 共9页
卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf_第2页
第2页 / 共9页
卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf_第3页
第3页 / 共9页
卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf_第4页
第4页 / 共9页
卷三 2018年6月英语六级真题及答案.pdf_第5页
第5页 / 共9页
亲,该文档总共9页,到这儿已超出免费预览范围,如果喜欢就下载吧!
资源描述

1、2018 年年 6 月大学英语六级真题及答案月大学英语六级真题及答案Part IWriting(30 minutes)Directions: For this part, you are allowed 30 minutes to write an essay on theimportance of building trust between teachers and students. You cancite examples to illustrate your views. You should write at least 150words but no more than 200

2、words.Part IIListening Comprehension(30 minutes)说明: 由于 2018 年 6 月六级考试全国共考了 2 套听力,本套真题听力与前 2 套内容完全一样,只是顺序不一样,因此在本套真题中不再重复出现。Part Reading Comprehension(40 minutes)Section ADirections: In this section, there is a passage with ten blanks. You are required toselect one word for each blank from a list of

3、choices given in a word bankfollowing the passage. Read the passage through carefully before makingyour choices, Each choice in the bank is identified by a letter. Pleasemark the corresponding letter for each item on Answer Sheet 2 with asingle line through the centre. You may not use any of the wor

4、ds in thebank more than once.Scientists scanning and mapping the Giza pyramids say theyve discovered thatthe Great Pyramid of Giza is not exactly even. But really not by much. This pyramidis the oldest of the world s Seven Wonders. The pyramids exact size has(26)_ experts for centuries, as the more

5、than 21 acres of hard, white casingstones that originally covered it were (27)_ long ago. Reporting in the mostrecent issue of the newsletter AERAGRAM,which (28)_ the work of theAncient Egypt Research Associates, engineer Glen Dash says his team used a newmeasuring approach that involved finding any

6、 surviving (29)_ of the casing inorder to determine where the original edge was. They found the east side of thepyramid to be a 30)_ of 5.5 inches shorter than the west side.The question that most 31)_ him, however, isnt how the Egyptians whodesigned and built the pyramid got it wrong 4,500 years ag

7、o, but how they got it sodose to 32)_ . We can only speculate as how the Egyptians could have laidout these lines with such (33)_ using only the tools they had, Dash writes. Hesays his 34)_ is that the Egyptians laid out their design on a grid, noting thatthe great pyramid is oriented only (35)_ awa

8、y from the cardinal directions (itsnorth-south axis runs 3 minutes 54 seconds west of due north,while its east-west axisruns 3 minutes 51 seconds north of due east)an amount thats tiny but similar,archeologistAtlas Obscura points out.A) chroniclesB) completeC) establishedD) fascinatesE) hypothesisF)

9、 maximumG) momentumH) mysteriouslyI)perfectJ) precisionK) puzzledL) remnantsM) removedN) revelationsO) slightlySection BDirections: In this section, you are going to read a passage with ten statementsattached to it. Each statement contains information given in one of theparagraphs. Identify the para

10、graph from which the information is derived.You may choose a paragraph more than once. Each paragraph is markedwith a letter. Answer the questions by marking the corresponding letteron Answer Sheet 2.Peer Pressure Has a Positive SideA Parents of teenagers often view their childrens friends with some

11、thing likesuspicion. They worry that the adolescent peer group has the power to push itsmembers into behavior that is foolish and even dangerous. Such wariness is wellfounded: statistics show, for example, that a teenage driver with a same-age passengerin the car is at higher risk of a fatal crash t

12、han an adolescent driving alone or with anadult.B In a 2005 study, psychologist Laurence Steinberg of Temple University andhis co-author, psychologist Margo Gardner, then at Temple, divided 306 people intothree age groups: young adolescents, with a mean age of 14; older adolescents, with amean age o

13、f 19; and adults, aged 24 and older. Subjects played a computerizeddriving game in which the player must avoid crashing into a wall that materializes,without warning, on the roadway. Steinberg and Gardner randomly assigned someparticipants to play alone or with two same-age peers looking on.C Older

14、adolescents scored about 50 percent higher on an index of riskydriving when their peers were in the roomand the driving of early adolescents wasfully twice as reckless when other young teens were around. In contrast, adultsbehaved in similar ways regardless of whether they were on their own or obser

15、ved byothers. The presence of peers makes adolescents and youth, but not adults, morelikely to take risks, Steinberg and Gardner concluded.D Yet in the years following the publication of this study, Steinberg began tobelieve that this interpretation did not capture the whole picture. As he and other

16、researchers examined the question of why teens were more apt to take risks in thecompany of other teenagers, they came to suspect that a crowd s influence need notalways be negative. Now some experts are proposing that we should take advantageof the teen brains keen sensitivity to the presence of fr

17、iends and leverage it toimprove education.E In a 2011 study, Steinberg and his colleagues turned to functional MRI (磁共振) to investigate how the presence of peers affects the activity in the adolescent brain.They scanned the brains of 40 teens and adults who were playing a virtual drivinggame designe

18、d to test whether players would brake at a yellow light or speed onthrough the crossroad.F The brains of teenagers, but not adults, showed greater activity in two regionsassociated with rewards when they were being observed by same-age peers than whenalone. In other words,rewards are more intense fo

19、r teens when they are with peers,which motivates them to pursue higher-risk experiences that might bring a big payoff(such as the thrill of just making the light before it turns red). But Steinberg suspectedthis tendency could also have its advantages. In his latest experiment, publishedonline in Au

20、gust, Steinberg and his colleagues used a computerized version of a cardgame called the Iowa Gambling Task to investigate how the presence of peers affectsthe way young people gather and apply information.G The results: Teens who played the Iowa Gambling Task under the eyes offellow adolescents enga

21、ged in more exploratory behavior,learned faster from bothpositive and negative outcomes,and achieved better performance on the task thanthose who played in solitude. What our study suggests is that teenagers learn morequickly and more effectively when their peers are present than when they re on the

22、irown, Steinberg says.And this finding could have important implications for how wethink about educating adolescents.H Matthew D. Lieberman, a social cognitive neuroscientist at the University ofCalifornia, Los Angeles, and author of the 2013 book Social: Why Our Brains AreWired to Connect,suspects

23、that the human brain is especially skillful at learningsocially significant information. He points to a classic 2004 study in whichpsychologists at Dartmouth College and Harvard University used functional MRI totrack brain activity in 17 young men as they listened to descriptions of people whileconc

24、entrating on either socially relevant cues ( for example , trying to form animpression of a person based on the description) or more socially neutral information(such as noting the order of details in the description). The descriptions were the samein each condition, but people could better remember

25、 these statements when given asocial motivation.I The study also found that when subjects thought about and later recalleddescriptions in terms of their informational content, regions associated with factualmemory, such as the medial temporal lobe, became active. But thinking about orremembering des

26、criptions in terms of their social meaning activated the dorsomedialprefrontal cortexpart of the brains social networkeven as traditional memoryregions registered low levels of activity. More recently, as he reported in a 2012review, Lieberman has discovered that this region may be part of a distinc

27、t networkinvolved in socially motivated learning and memory. Such findings, he says, suggestthat this network can be called on to process and store the kind of information taughtin schoolpotentially giving students access to a range of untapped mental powers.J If humans are generally geared to recal

28、l details about one another, this patternis probably even more powerful among teenagers who are very attentive to socialdetails: who is in, who is out, who likes whom, who is mad at whom. Their desire forsocial drama is notor not onlya way of distracting themselves from theirschoolwork or of driving

29、 adults crazy. It is actually a neurological (神经的) sensitivity,initiated by hormonal changes. Evolutionarily speaking, people in this age group areat a stage in which they can prepare to find a mate and start their own family whileseparating from parents and striking out on their own. To do this suc

30、cessfully, theirbrain prompts them to think and even obsess about others.K Yet our schools focus primarily on students as individual entities. Whatwould happen if educators instead took advantage of the fact that teens are powerfullycompelled to think in social terms? In Social, Lieberman lays out a

31、 number of ways todo so. History and English could be presented through the lens of the psychologicaldrives of the people involved. One could therefore present Napoleon in terms of hisdesire to impress or Churchill in terms of his lonely gloom. Less inherentlyinterpersonal subjects, such as math, co

32、uld acquire a social aspect through teamproblem solving and peer tutoring. Research shows that when we absorb informationin order to teach it to someone else, we learn it more accurately and deeply, perhaps inpart because we are engaging our social cognition.L And although anxious parents may not we

33、lcome the notion, educators couldturn adolescent recklessness to academic ends. Risk taking in an educational contextis a vital skill that-enables progress and creativity, wrote Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, acognitive neuroscientist at University College London, in a review published last year.Yet, she no

34、ted, many young people are especially unwilling to take risks atschoolafraid that one low test score or poor grade could cost them a spot at aselective university. We should assure such students that risk, and even peer pressure,can be a good thingas long as it happens in the classroom and not in th

35、e car.36. It is thought probable that the human brain is particularly good at picking upsocially important information.37. It can be concluded from experiments that the presence of peers increasesrisk-taking by adolescents and youth.38. Students should be told that risk-taking in the classroom can b

36、e somethingpositive.39. The urge of finding a mate and getting married accounts for adolescentsgreater attention to social interactions.40. According to Steinberg, the presence of peers increases the speed andeffectiveness of teenagerslearning.41. Teenagers parents are often concerned about negative

37、 peer influence.42. Activating the brains social network involved in socially motivated learningand memory may allow students to tap unused mental powers.43. The presence of peers intensifies the feeling of rewards in teens brains.44. When we absorb information for the purpose of imparting it to oth

38、ers, we doso with greater accuracy and depth.45. Some experts are suggesting that we turn peer influence to good use ineducation.Section CDirections: There are 2 passages in this section. Each passage is followed by somequestions or unfinished statements. For each of them there are fourchoices marke

39、d A), B), C) and D). You should decide on the best choiceand mark the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2with a single linethrough the centre.Passage OneQuestions 46 to 50 are based on the following passage.The Ebro Delta, in Spain, famous as a battleground during the Spanish Civil War,is now the

40、 setting for a different contest, one that is pitting rice farmers against twoenemies:the rice-eating giant apple snail, and rising sea levels. What happens herewill have a bearing on the future of European rice production and the overall health ofsouthern European wetlands.Located on the Mediterran

41、ean just two hours south of Barcelona, the Ebro Deltaproduces 120 million kilograms of rice a year, making it one of the continents mostimportant rice-growing areas. As sea creeps into these fresh-water marshes, however,rising salinity (盐分)is hampering rice production. At the same time, this sea-wat

42、eralso kills off the greedy giant apple snail, an introduced pest that feeds on young riceplants. The most promising strategy has become to harness one foe against the other.The battle is currently being waged on land, in greenhouses at the University ofBarcelona. Scientists working under the banner

43、 Project Neurice are seekingvarieties of rice that can withstand the increasing salinity without losing theabsorbency that makes European rice ideal for traditional Spanish and Italian dishes.The project has two sides,says Xavier Serrat,Neurice project manager andresearcher at the University of Barc

44、elona, the short-term fight against the snail, and amid-to long-term fight against climate change. But the snail has given the projectgreater urgency. Originally from South America, the snails were accidentally introduced into theEbro Delta by Global Aquatic Technologies, a company that raised the s

45、nails forfresh-water aquariums (水族馆) , but failed to prevent their escape. For now, the giantapple snails presence in Europe is limited to the Ebro Delta. But the snail continuesits march to new territory, says Serrat. The question is not whether it will reach otherrice-growing areas of Europe, but

46、when.Over the next year and a half investigators will test the various strains ofsalt-tolerant rice theyve bred. In 2018, farmers will plant the varieties with the mostpromise in the Ebro Delta and Europes other two main rice-growing regionsalongthe Po in Italy, and Frances Rhone. A season in the fi

47、eld will help determine which, ifany, of the varieties are ready for commercialization.As an EU-funded effort, the search for salt-tolerant varieties of rice is takingplace in all three countries. Each team is crossbreeding a local European short-grainrice with a long-grain Asian variety that carrie

48、s the salt-resistant gene. The scientistsare breeding successive generations to arrive at varieties that incorporate salttolerance but retain about 97 percent of the European rice genome (基因组).46. Why does the author mention the Spanish Civil War at the beginning of thepassage?A) It had great impact

49、 on the life of Spanish rice farmers.B) It is of great significance in the records of Spanish history.C) Rice farmers in the Ebro Delta are waging a battle of similar importance.D) Rice farmers in the Ebro Delta are experiencing as hard a time as in the war.47. what may be the most effective strateg

50、y for rice farmers to employ infighting their enemies?A) Striking the weaker enemy first.B) Killing two birds with one stone.C) Eliminating the enemy one by one.D) Using one evil to combat the other.48. What do we learn about Project Neurice?A) Its goals will have to be realized at a cost.B) It aims

展开阅读全文
相关资源
相关搜索
资源标签

当前位置:首页 > 教育专区 > 高中资料

本站链接:文库   一言   我酷   合作


客服QQ:2549714901微博号:文库网官方知乎号:文库网

经营许可证编号: 粤ICP备2021046453号世界地图

文库网官网©版权所有2025营业执照举报